Thinkering through Experiments: Nurturing Transdisciplinary Approaches to the Design of Testing Tools

Kathryn B. Francis, Agi Haines, Raluca A. Briazu

Abstract


In order to assess and understand human behavior, traditional approaches to experimental design incorporate testing tools that are often artificial and devoid of corporeal features. Whilst these offer experimental control in situations in which, methodologically, real behaviors cannot be examined, there is increasing evidence that responses given in these contextually deprived experiments fail to trigger genuine responses. This may result from a lack of consideration regarding the material makeup and associations connected with the fabric of experimental tools. In a twoyear collaboration, we began to experiment with the physicality of testing tools using the domain of moral psychology as a case study. This collaboration involved thinkering and prototyping methods that included direct contact and consideration of the materials involved in experimentation. Having explored the embodied nature of morality, we combined approaches from experimental psychology, moral philosophy, design thinking, and computer science to create a new testing tool for simulated moral behavior. Although the testing tool itself generated fruitful results, this paper considers the collaborative methodology through which it was produced as a route to highlight material questions within psychological research.

Keywords


collaboration; embodiment; methodology; moral psychology; prototyping; thinkering

Full Text:

PDF

References


Antonelli, P. (2011) States of design 03: Thinkering. Domus, 948. Retrieved from: http://www.domusweb.it/en/design/2011/07/04/states-of-design-03-thinkering.html

Baudrillard, J. (1994). Simulacra and simulation. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

Briazu, R. A., Francis, K. B., & Haines, A. (2015, September). The affective embodiment of testing tools and their influence on experimental outcomes. Paper presented at the Off The Lip - Trandisciplinary Approaches to Cognitive Innovation, Plymouth, UK. Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.net/10026.1/4271

Brown, J. S. (2009). Learning 2.0: The big picture [PDF presentation]. Retrieved from http://www.johnseelybrown.com/learning2.pdf

Christensen, J. F., Flexas, A., Calabrese, M., Gut, N. K., & Gomila, A. (2014). Moral judgment reloaded: A moral dilemma validation study. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–18. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00607

Epstein, S. L. (2005). Making interdisciplinary collaboration work. In S. J. Derry, C. D. Schunn, & M. A. Gernsbacher (Eds.), Interdisciplinary collaboration: An emerging cognitive science (pp. 245–263). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Foot, P. (1978). Virtues and vices and other essays in moral philosophy. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Francis, K. B., Howard, C., Howard, I. S., Gummerum, M., Ganis, G., Anderson, G., & Terbeck, S. (2016). Virtual morality: Transitioning from moral judgment to moral action? PLoS One, 11(10), 1–22. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0164374

Francis, K. B., Terbeck, S., Briazu, R. A., Haines, A., Gummerum, M., Ganis, G., & Howard, I. S. (2017). Simulating moral actions: An investigation of personal force in virtual moral dilemmas. Scientific Reports, 7. doi:10.1038/s41598-017-13909-9

Innes, J. E., & Booher, D. E. (1999). Consensus building as role playing and bricolage: Toward a theory of collaborative planning. Journal of the American Planning Association, 65, 9–26.

Ingold, T. (2010). The textility of making. Cambridge Journal of Economics, 34, 91–102.

Ingold, T. (2013). Making: Anthropology, archaeology, art and architecture. Oxford, UK: Routledge.

Kline, P. (2015). A handbook of test construction: Introduction to psychometric design. New York, NY: Routledge.

Latour, B. (1990). Visualisation and cognition: Thinking with eyes and hands. In M. E. Lynch & S. Woolgar (Eds.), Representation in Scientific Practice. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Moro, C. (2015). Material culture: Still ‘terra incognita’ for psychology today? Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 11(2), 172–176. doi:10.5964/ejop.v11i2.995

Patil, I., Cogoni, C., Zangrando, N., Chittaro, L., & Silani, G. (2014). Affective basis of judgmentbehavior discrepancy in virtual experiences of moral dilemmas. Social Neuroscience, 9(1), 94–107. doi:10.1080/17470919.2013.870091

Peralta, C., & Moultrie, J. (2010). Collaboration between designers and scientists in the context of scientific research: A literature review. In D. Marjanovic, M. Storga, N. Pavovic, & N. Bojcetic (Eds.), DS 60: Proceedings of DESIGN 2010, the 11th International Design Conference, Dubrovnik, Croatia (pp. 1643-1652). Glasgow, UK: The Design Society. Retrieved from https://www.designsociety.org/publication/29510/collaboration_between_designers_and_scientists_in_the_context_of_scientific_research_a_literature_review

Piaget, J. (1976). Piaget’s theory. In B. Inhelder, H. H. Chipman, & C. Zwingmann (Eds.), Piaget and his school: A reader in delevopmental psychology (pp. 11–23). New Yor, NY: Springer-Verlag.

Small, C. (1998). Musicking. Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press. Terbeck, S., & Francis, K. B. (in press). We should if we could, but we can't. Experimental problems with moral enhancement. In M. Hauskeller (Ed.), Moral Enhancement. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Thomson, J. J. (1976). Killing, letting die, and the trolley problem. Monist, 59(2), 204–217.


Refbacks

  • There are currently no refbacks.


Copyright (c) 2018 Kathryn B. Francis, Agi Haines, Raluca A. Briazu