Avant, Vol. X, No. 2/2019, doi: 10.26913/avant.2019.02.01
published under license CC BY-NC-ND 3.0
Aneta Niczyporuk
Institute of Sociology and Cognitive Science
University of Bialystok, Poland
aneta.niczyporuk @ uwb.edu.pl
Received 1 September 2018; accepted 23 October 2018; published 18 September 2019. Download full text
Abstract: Research reveals a unique relationship between image representations and emotions. In this context, intrusive mental images could be especially unpleasant and distressing. On the other hand, because of their impact on emotions, images can serve as a support to self-regulation, including regulation of one’s own thoughts. In the article, a thought suppression strategy is proposed that uses images to fight harmful intrusions. This proposition is based on several research areas: studies on image representations properties, the “think/no-think” and the “white bear” thought suppression paradigms, and Lavie’s attention theory The strategy assumes the redirection of attention from unwanted content by using image representations in conditions of maximally freed-up cognitive control resources.
Keywords: thought suppression; intrusions; cognitive control; load; image representations
References
Abramowitz, J. S., Whiteside, S., Kalsy, S. A., & Tolin, D. F. (2003). Thought control strategies in obsessive-compulsive disorder: A replication and extension. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 529-540. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00026-8 |
||||
Abramowitz, Jonathan S., Tolin, D. F., & Street, G. P. (2001). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression: A meta-analysis of controlled studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 683-703. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(00)00057-X |
||||
Amir, N., Cashman, L., & Foa, E. B. (1997). Strategies of thought control in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 775-777. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00030-2 |
||||
Anderson, M. C., & Green, C. (2001). Suppressing unwanted memories by executive control. Nature, 410, 366-369. https://doi.org/10.1038/35066572 |
||||
Anderson, Michael C., & Huddleston, E. (2012). Towards a cognitive and neurobiological model of motivated forgetting. In R. F. Belli (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation: Vol. 58. True and false recovered memories: Toward a reconciliation of the debate (pp. 53-120). New York, NY, US: Springer Science + Business Media. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-1195-6_3 |
||||
Anderson, M. C., Ochsner, K. N., Kuhl, B., Cooper, J., Robertson, E., Gabrieli, S. W., Glover, G. H., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2004). Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted memories. Science, 303, 232-235. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1089504 |
||||
Anderson, Michael C., Reinholz, J., Kuhl, B. A., & Mayr, U. (2011). Intentional suppression of unwanted memories grows more difficult as we age. Psychology and Aging, 26, 397-405. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022505 |
||||
Andrade, J., May, J., & Kavanagh, D. (2012). Sensory imagery in craving: From cognitive psychology to new treatments for addiction. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 3, 127-145. https://doi.org/10.5127/jep.024611 |
||||
Arntz, A., De Groot, C., & Kindt, M. (2005). Emotional memory is perceptual. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 36, 19-34. | ||||
Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1-29. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422 |
||||
Blackwell, S. E., Rius-Ottenheim, N., Schulte-van Maaren, Y. W., Carlier, I. V., Middelkoop, V. D., Zitman, F. G., Spinhoven, P., Holmes, E. A., & Giltay, E. J. (2013). Optimism and mental imagery: A possible cognitive marker to promote well-being?. Psychiatry Research, 206, 56-61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.09.047 |
||||
Brewin, C. R., Gregory, J. D., Lipton, M., & Burgess, N. (2010). Intrusive images in psychological disorders: characteristics, neural mechanisms, and treatment implications. Psychological Review, 117, 210-232. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018113 |
||||
Burnett Heyes, S., Lau, J. Y. F., & Holmes, E. A. (2013). Mental imagery, emotion and psychopathology across child and adolescent development. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 119-133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2013.02.004 |
||||
Çili, S., & Stopa, L. (2015). Intrusive mental imagery in psychological disorders: Is the self the key to understanding maintenance? Frontiers in Psychiatry, 6, 103-107. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00103 |
||||
Conway, M. A. (2001). Sensory-perceptual episodic memory and its context: Autobiographical memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 356, 1375-1384. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2001.0940 |
||||
Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107, 261-288. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.107.2.261 |
||||
Depue, B. E., Curran, T., & Banich, M. T. (2007). Prefrontal regions orchestrate suppression of emotional memories via a two-phase process. Science, 317, 215-219. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1139560 |
||||
Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2007). High perceptual load makes everybody equal. Psychological Science, 18, 377-381. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01908.x |
||||
Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2009). Harnessing the wandering mind: The role of perceptual load. Cognition, 111, 345-355. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.02.006 |
||||
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2017). Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex, 86, 186-204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.04.023 |
||||
Harvey, A. G., & Payne, S. (2002). The management of unwanted pre-sleep thoughts in insomnia: Distraction with imagery versus general distraction. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 267-277. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00012-2 |
||||
Hertel, P. T., & Calcaterra, G. (2005). Intentional forgetting benefits from thought substitution. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12, 484-489. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193792 |
||||
Holmes, E. A., Lang, T. J., & Shah, D. M. (2009). Developing interpretation bias modification as a “cognitive vaccine” for depressed mood: Imagining positive events makes you feel better than thinking about them verbally. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 76-88. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012590 |
||||
Holmes, E. A., & Mathews, A. (2005). Mental imagery and emotion: A special relationship? Emotion, 5, 489-497. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.4.489 |
||||
Holmes, E. A., & Mathews, A. (2010). Mental imagery in emotion and emotional disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 349-362. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.01.001 |
||||
Holmes, E. A., Mathews, A., Dalgleish, T., & Mackintosh, B. (2006). Positive interpretation training: Effects of mental imagery versus verbal training on positive mood. Behavior Therapy, 37, 237-247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2006.02.002 |
||||
Holmes, E. A., Mathews, A., Mackintosh, B., & Dalgleish, T. (2008). The causal effect of mental imagery on emotion assessed using picture-word cues. Emotion, 8, 395-409. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.395 |
||||
Joormann, J., Hertel, P. T., LeMoult, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2009). Training forgetting of negative material in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 34-43. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013794 |
||||
Ju, Y. J., & Lien, Y. W. (2016). Better control with less effort: The advantage of using focused-breathing strategy over focused-distraction strategy on thought suppression. Consciousness and Cognition, 40, 9-16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.12.002 |
||||
Lavie, N. (2010). Attention, distraction, and cognitive control under load. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 143-148. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721410370295 |
||||
Lavie, N., Hirst, A., De Fockert, J. W., & Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 339-354. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.339 |
||||
Levy, B. J., & Anderson, M. C. (2008). Individual differences in the suppression of unwanted memories: The executive deficit hypothesis. Acta Psychologica, 127, 623-635. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.12.004 |
||||
Lin, Y. J., & Wicker, F. W. (2007). A comparison of the effects of thought suppression, distraction and concentration. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2924-2937. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2007.08.004 |
||||
Luciano, J. V., & González, S. A. (2007). Analysis of the efficacy of different thought suppression strategies. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 7, 335-345. | ||||
Macrae, C. N., Milne, A. B., Bodenhausen, G. V., & Ford, R. L. (1997). On the regulation of recollection: The intentional forgetting of stereotypical memories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 709-719. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.72.4.709 |
||||
Mathews, A., Ridgeway, V., & Holmes, E. A. (2013). Feels like the real thing: Imagery is both more realistic and emotional than verbal thought. Cognition and Emotion, 27, 217-229. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2012.698252 |
||||
May, J., Andrade, J., Willoughby, K., & Brown, C. (2012). An attentional control task reduces intrusive thoughts about smoking. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 14, 472-478. https://doi.org/10.1093/ntr/ntr238 |
||||
Miller, G. A., Levin, D. N., Kozak, M. J., Edwin, E., McLean, A., & Lang, P. J. (1987). Individual differences in imagery and the psychophysiology of emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 367−390. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699938708408058 |
||||
Moors, A., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Automaticity: A theoretical and conceptual analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 297-326. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.297 |
||||
Murray, B. D., Anderson, M. C., & Kensinger, E. A. (2015). Older adults can suppress unwanted memories when given an appropriate strategy. Psychology and Aging, 30, 9-25. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038611 |
||||
Nigg, J. T. (2017). Annual Research Review: On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 58, 361-383. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12675 |
||||
Nixon, R. D. V., Cain, N., Nehmy, T., & Seymour, M. (2009). The influence of thought suppression and cognitive load on intrusions and memory processes following an analogue stressor. Behavior Therapy, 40, 368-379. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2008.10.004 |
||||
Nixon, R. D. V., & Rackebrandt, J. (2016). Cognitive load undermines thought suppression in acute stress disorder. Behavior Therapy, 47, 388-403. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2016.02.010 |
||||
Noreen, S., & De Fockert, J. W. (2017). The role of cognitive load in intentional forgetting using the think/no-think task. Experimental Psychology, 64, 14-26. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000347 |
||||
Oberauer, K. (2009). Design for a working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 51, 45-100. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-394293-7.00002-9 |
||||
Paz-Alonso, P. M., Ghetti, S., Matlen, B. J., Anderson, M. C., & Bunge, S. A. (2009). Memory suppression is an active process that improves over childhood. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 3, 24. https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.09.024.2009 |
||||
Pearson, J., Naselaris, T., Holmes, E. A., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2015). Mental imagery: Functional mechanisms and clinical applications. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 590-602. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.08.003 |
||||
Rachman, S., & de Silva, P. (1978). Abnormal and normal obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 16, 233-248. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(78)90022-0 |
||||
Rassin, E. (2005). Thought Suppression. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-044714-8.X5028-4 |
||||
Salkovskis, P. M., & Reynolds, M. (1994). Thought suppression and smoking cessation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 193-201. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(94)90112-0 |
||||
Teasdale, J. D., Dritschel, B. H., Taylor, M. J., Proctor, L., Lloyd, C. A., Nimmo-Smith, I., & Baddeley, A. D. (1995). Stimulus-independent thought depends on central executive resources. Memory & Cognition, 23, 551-559. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197257 |
||||
Teasdale, J. D., Proctor, L., Lloyd, C. A., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Working memory and stimulus-independent thought: Effects of memory load and presentation rate. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 5, 417-433. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541449308520128 |
||||
van Den Hout, M., Muris, P., Salemink, E., & Kindt, M. (2001). Autobiographical memories become less vivid and emotional after eye movements. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 40, 121-130. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466501163571 |
||||
Versland, A., & Rosenberg, H. (2007). Effect of brief imagery interventions on craving in college student smokers. Addiction Research and Theory, 15, 177-187. https://doi.org/10.1080/16066350701200582 |
||||
Vrana, S. R., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (1986). Fear imagery and text processing. Psychophysiology, 23, 247-253. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00626.x |
||||
Wang, D., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Hagger, M. S. (2017). Mechanisms underlying effective thought suppression using focused-distraction strategies: A self-determination theory approach. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory Research, and Practice 4, 367-380. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000138 |
||||
Wang, D., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Hagger, M. S. (2018). Why distractors with need-supportive content can mitigate ironic effects of thought suppression. Motivation and Emotion, 42, 214-224. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-017-9653-3 |
||||
Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34-52. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.101.1.34 |
||||
Wegner, D. M., & Erber, R. (1992). The hyperaccessibility of suppressed thoughts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 903-912. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.6.903 |
||||
Wegner, D. M., Erber, R., & Zanakos, S. (1993). Ironic processes in the mental control of mood and mood-related thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1093-1104. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.6.1093 |
||||
Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 5-13. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.1.5 |
||||
Wenzlaff, R. M., & Bates, D. E. (2000). The relative efficacy of concentration and suppression strategies of mental control. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1200-1212. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167200262003 |
||||
Wenzlaff, R. M., & Wegner, D. M. (2000). Thought suppression. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 59-91. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.59 |
||||
Whitaker, K. L., Watson, M., & Brewin, C. R. (2009). Intrusive cognitions and their appraisal in anxious cancer patients. Psycho-Oncology, 18, 1147-1155. https://doi.org/10.1002/pon.1512 |