A Social-relational Perspective on Social Cognitive Development
Avant, Vol. X, No. 3/2019, doi: 10.26913/avant.2019.03.28
published under license CC BY-NC-ND 3.0
Duygu Uygun Tunc
University of Heidelberg
duygu.uygun @ outlook.com
Published Online First 31 December 2019 Download full text
Abstract: It is widely agreed that perspective-taking plays an important role in the development of children’s understanding of themselves and others as social agents with their own beliefs, desires, goals, and representations of the world. However, how perspective-taking is realized and how the ability of perspective-taking develops is a matter of dissensus. The two theories currently dominating social cognition research, theory-theory, and simulation-theory construe perspective-taking as modeling, thus as an individual and inferential process. Interactionist theories prioritize interpersonal interaction but deny perspective-taking a constitutive role by arguing for a basic, immediate understanding of self and others in interaction. Cognitivist accounts downplay the role of interaction, while interactionist accounts overemphasize the role of sub-symbolical processes. What is central to perspective-taking and its development, but missing in either approach is symbolically mediated interaction. The social-relational perspective dating back to Lev Vygotsky and George Herbert Mead cuts across this schism and offers valuable insight into how perspective-taking develops through symbolic activity within a social context. Adopting the basic elements of the social-relational framework, the present work argues that understanding of self and others depends on the development of perspective-taking ability through symbolically mediated interaction. Perspectives are primarily differentiated, assumed, and coordinated within social interaction and subsequently through the individual, cognitive operation of perspective-taking. Symbolic mediation facilitates this transition from the social enaction of perspective-taking to mental construal and coordination of perspectives by transforming the structure of action. Higher order mental processes are not presupposed but constituted by social interaction through the child’s internalization of the perspectival structure of symbolic communication.
Keywords: cperspective-taking; symbolic interaction; cognitive development; social interaction; cognition
References
Astington, J., Harris, P. L., Olson, D. R.(Eds.). (1988). Developing Theories of Mind. Cambridge University Press. | ||||
Baldwin, J.M. (1906/ 1894). Mental development in the child and the race: Methods and processes (3rd rev. ed.). New York: Macmillan https://doi.org/10.1037/12382-000 |
||||
Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H.M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. New York: Oxford University Press. | ||||
Baron-Cohen, S., & Ring, H. (1994). A model of the mindreading system: Neuropsychological and neurobiological perspectives. In C. Lewis & P. Mitchell (Eds.), Children’s early understanding of mind: Origins and development (pp. 183-207). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates | ||||
Blumer, H. (1969). Symbolic interactionism: Perspective and method. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. | ||||
Bruner, J. S. (1990). Acts of meaning (Vol. 3). Cambridge: Harvard University Press. | ||||
Carpendale, J., & Lewis, C. (2006). How children develop social understanding. Oxford: Blackwell. | ||||
Carruthers, P. (2009). How we know our own minds: The relationship between mindreading and metacognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(2), 121-38. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X09000545 |
||||
Clark, E. V. (1997). Conceptual perspective and lexical choice in acquisition. Cognition, 64 (1), 1-37. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00010-3 |
||||
Cole, M. (1985). The zone of proximal development: where culture and cognition create each other. in J.V. Wertsch (ed.), Culture, Communication and Cognition: Vygotskian Perspectives (pp. 146-161). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | ||||
Cole, M., Engeström, Y., Vasquez, O. (1997). Mind, culture and activity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327884mca0403_6 https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327884mca0403_4 |
||||
Côté, J-F. (2016). George Herbert Mead’s concept of society: A critical reconstruction. NY: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315634517 |
||||
De Jaegher, H., & Di Paolo, E. (2007). Participatory sense- making: An enactive approach to social cognition. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6(4), 485-507. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-007-9076-9 |
||||
De Jaegher, H., Di Paolo, E., & Gallagher, S. (2010). Can Social Interaction Constitute Social Cognition? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(10), 441-447. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.06.009 |
||||
De Jaegher, H. & Froese, T. (2009). On the role of social interaction in individual agency. Adaptive Behavior, 17 (5), 444-460. https://doi.org/10.1177/1059712309343822 |
||||
Edwards, A. (2007). An interesting resemblance. In H. Daniels, M. Cole, & J. V. Wertsch (Eds.), The Cambridge companion to Vygotsky (pp. 77-100). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CCOL0521831040.004 |
||||
Flavell, J. H. (2000). Development of children’s knowledge about the mental world. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24(1), 15-23. https://doi.org/10.1080/016502500383421 |
||||
Flavell, J. H., & Miller, P. H. (1998). Social cognition. In D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and language (5th ed.), (pp. 851-898). New York: Wiley | ||||
Fuchs, T., & De Jaegher, H. (2009). Enactive intersubjectivity: Participatory sense-making and mutual incorporation. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 8(4), 465-486. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-009-9136-4 |
||||
Gallagher, S. (2001). The practice of mind: Theory, simulation, or primary interaction? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8 (5-7), 83-108. | ||||
Gallagher, S. (2008). Direct perception in the intersubjective context. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(2), 535-543. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.003 |
||||
Gallagher, S. & Hutto, D. (2008). Understanding others through Primary Interaction and Narrative Practice. In J. Zlatev, T. Racine, C. Sinha, & E. Itkonen (Eds.), The Shared Mind: Perspectives on Intersubjectivity (pp. 17-38). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/celcr.12.04gal |
||||
Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mindreading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(12), 493-501. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01262-5 |
||||
Gallese, V. (2014). Bodily selves in relation: embodied simulation as second-person perspective on intersubjectivity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1644). https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0177 |
||||
Gillespie, A. (2005). G.H. Mead: Theorist of the social act. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 35(1), 19-39. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0021-8308.2005.00262.x |
||||
Goldman, A. I. (2006). Simulating minds: The philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience of mindreading. Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/0195138929.001.0001 |
||||
Gopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1997). Words, thoughts, and theories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | ||||
Gordon, R. M. (1986). Folk psychology as simulation. Mind and Language, 1(2), 158-171. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.1986.tb00324.x |
||||
Martin, J., Sokol, B. W., & Elfers, T. (2008). Taking and coordinating perspectives: From prereflective interactivity, through reflective intersubjectivity, to metareflective sociality. Human Development, 51(5-6), 294-317. https://doi.org/10.1159/000170892 |
||||
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. | ||||
Mead, G. H. (1972). The philosophy of the act. London: University of Chicago Press. | ||||
Mounoud, P. (1996). Perspective taking and belief attribution: From Piaget’s theory to children’s theory of mind. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 55(2/3), 93-103. | ||||
Nelson, K. (1998). Language in cognitive development: The emergence of the mediated mind. Cambridge University Press. | ||||
Peirce, C. S. (1931-58). The collected papers of Charles Sanders Peirce (Vol. 1-6 edited by C. Hartshorne & P. Weiss, volumes 7&8 edited by A. W. Burks). Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press (cited as CP). | ||||
Peirce, C. S. (1992). The essential Peirce: selected philosophical writings (Vol. 2). Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press (cited as EP). | ||||
Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | ||||
Perner, J., Stummer, S., & Lang, B. (1999). Executive functions and theory of mind: Cognitive complexity or functional dependence? In P. D. Zelazo, J. W. Astington, & D. R. Olson (Eds.), Developing theories of intention: Social understanding and self-control (pp. 133-152). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | ||||
Perner, J., Stummer, S., Sprung, M., & Doherty, M. (2002). Theory of mind finds its Piagetian perspective: Why alternative naming comes with understanding belief. Cognitive Development, 17(3-4), 1451-1472. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00127-2 |
||||
Piaget, J. (1928). Judgment and reasoning of the child. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. | ||||
Piaget, J. (1962/1945). Play, Dreams and Imitation in Childhood. New York: Norton. | ||||
Piaget, J. (1977/1974). The grasp of consciousness: Action and concept in the young child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. | ||||
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1963/1948). The child’s conception of space. London: Routledge & Kegan. | ||||
Reddy, V., & Morris, P. (2004). Participants don’t need theories: Knowing minds in engagement. Theory & Psychology, 14(5), 647-665. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354304046177 |
||||
Rogoff, B. (1999). Cognitive development through social interaction: Vygotsky and Piaget. In P. Murphy (Ed.), Learners, Learning and Assessment (pp. 69-82). London: Paul Chapman Publishing. | ||||
Rogoff, B. (2003). The Cultural Nature of Human Development. New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press. | ||||
Scholl, B. J. & Leslie, A. M. (1999). Modularity, development and ‘theory of mind’. Mind & Language. 14(1), 131-153. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0017.00106 |
||||
Shanton, K., & Goldman, A. (2010). Simulation theory. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(4), 527-538. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.33 |
||||
Shantz, C. U. (1983). Social cognition. In J. H. Flavell & E. M. Markman (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Cognitive development (pp. 495-555). New York: Wiley. | ||||
Varela, F., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/6730.001.0001 |
||||
Vygotsky, L. (1978/1930). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. | ||||
Vygotsky, L. (1987/1934). Thinking and speech. In R. W. Rieber & A. S. Carton (Eds.), The Collected Works of L. S. Vygotsky. Vol 1: Problems of General Psychology. New York: Plenum. | ||||
Wertsch, J. V. (1998). Mind as Action. Cambridge, MA: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195117530.001.0001 “Avant” journal – the task financed under the contract 711/P-DUN/2019 from the funds of the Minister of Science and Higher Education for the dissemination of science.Czasopismo „Avant” – zadanie finansowane w ramach umowy 711/P-DUN/2019 ze środków Ministra Nauki i Szkolnictwa Wyższego przeznaczonych na działalność upowszechniającą naukę. |