Motor Simulation and Ostensive-Inferential Communication

Avant, Vol. XIII, No. 1, https://doi.org/10.26913/ava202204
published under license CC BY-NC-ND 3.0

Angelo D. Delliponti
Academia Copernicana,
Nicolaus Copernicus University
angelo.d.delliponti@gmail.com

Received 9 September 2021; accepted 19 April 2022; published 17 June 2022.
Download full text


Abstract: The ostensive-inferential model is a model of communication, an alternative to the code model of communication, based on pragmatic competence: it explains human communication in terms of expression and recognition of informative and communicative intentions, founding comprehension on the distinction between literal meaning and the speaker’s meaning. Through informative intentions we try to make evident the content of a message to a receiver, or to make evident what we want to communicate to him/her: communicative intentions are used to make evident the very fact that we intend to communicate. One hypothesis is that ostensive-inferential communication is what makes human language possible. Since an extensive literature has highlighted the role of the Theory of Mind in ostensive-inferential communication, this hypothesis fits with the idea that a mechanism for mentalizing underlies human communication. The aim of the present paper is to stress the role of lower-level mechanisms, specifically of motor simulation, in the recognition of informative and communicative intentions, in order to outline an embodied account of ostensive communication. Specifically, the hypothesis is that this process is involved in language acquisition during development, and that it plays a role in the associative learning process involved in language acquisition during childhood. To this aim, in future research it may be useful to test the involvement of motor simulation (specifically, phono-articulatory and semantic) in the recognition of informative and communicative intentions in toddlers. Since some models of language evolution focus on the role of motor simulation, a supplementary goal is to deepen its role in the biological evolution of language, focusing on the specific link between motor simulation and intentions in the framework of ostensive-inferential model.

Keywords: ostensive communication; motor simulation; communicative intention; informative intention; embodiment; language acquisition


Bibliography

Angrave L.C., Glenberg A.M. (2007). Infant Gestures Predict Verb Production One Year Later. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association.
https://doi.org/10.1037/e705442007-001
Antognini K., Daum M. M. (2019) . Toddlers show sensorimotor activity during auditory verb processing. Neuropsychologia, 126, 82-91.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.07.022
Apperly I.A. (2011). Mindreaders: the cognitive basis of “theory of mind”. Psychology Press, New York.
https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203833926
Arbib M.A. (2012). How the brain got language: The mirror system hypothesis, New York (NY), Oxford University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:osobl/9780199896684.001.0001
Arbib M.A. (2013). Complex imitation and the language-ready brain. Language and Cognition, 5, 273-312.
https://doi.org/10.1515/langcog-2013-0020
Arbib M.A., Rizzolatti G. (1997). Neural expectations: A possible evolutionary path from manual skills to language. Communication and Cognition, 29, 393-424.
Armstrong, D. F., Stokoe,W. C., & Wilcox, S. E. (1995). Gesture and the nature of language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511620911
Aziz-Zadeh L., Wilson S.M., Rizzolatti G., Iacoboni M. (2006). Embodied semantics and the premotor cortex: Congruent representations for visually presented actions and linguistic phrases describing actions. Current Biology, 16, 1818-1823.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.060
Bak T.H., Hodges J.R. (2003). The effects of motor neurone disease on language: Further evidence. Brain and Language, 89, 354-361.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00357-2
Bambini V. (2017). Il cervello pragmatico, Roma, Carocci.
Bernardis P., Gentilucci M. (2006). Speech and gesture share the same communication system. Neuropsychologia, 44, 178-190.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.05.007
Binkofski F., Buccino G. (2006). The role of ventral premotor cortex in action execution and action understanding. J. Physiol. Paris 99, 396-405.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2006.03.005
Borghi A., Caruana F. (2016). Il cervello in azione, Bologna, Il Mulino.
Buccino G., Binkofski F., Fink G.R., Fadiga L., Fogassi L., Freund H.J., et al. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: An fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13(2), 400-404.
https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.2001.01385.x
Buccino G. Binkofski F., Riggio L. (2004b). The mirror neuron system and action recognition. Brain and Language, 89(2), 370-376.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00356-0
Buccino G., Riggio L., Melli G., Binkofski F., Gallese V., Rizzolatti G. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: a combined TMS and behavioral study. Cogn. Brain Res. 24, 355-363.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.02.020
Buccino G., Solodkin A., Small S. (2006). Functions of the mirror neuron system: Implications for neurorehabilitation. Cognitive Behavioral Neurology, 19, 55-63.
https://doi.org/10.1097/00146965-200603000-00007
Byom L. J. & Mutlu B. (2013). Theory of mind: Mechanisms, methods, and new directions. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 7, 413.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00413
Cavallo, A., Lungu, O., Becchio, C., Ansuini, C., Rustichini, A., & Fadiga, L. (2015). When gaze opens the channel for communication: Integrative role of IFG and MPFC. NeuroImage, 119, 63-69.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.025
Chomsky N. (1957). Syntactic structures, Princeton, Mouton and Co.
https://doi.org/10.1515/9783112316009
Cole, E.J., Barraclough, N.E., Andrews, T.J. (2019). Reduced connectivity between mentalizing and mirror systems in autism spectrum condition. Neuropsychologia 122, 88-97.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.11.008
Cook R., Bird G., Catmur C., Press C., Heyes C. (2014). Mirror neurons: from origin to function. Behav. Brain Sci. 37, 177-192.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X13000903
Cooper, R. P., Cook, R., Dickinson, A., & Heyes, C. M. (2013). Associative (not Hebbian) learning and the mirror neuron system. Neuroscience Letters, 540, 28-36.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2012.10.002
Corballis, M. C. (2002). From hand to mouth: The origins of language. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1515/9780691221731
Csibra G. (2007). Action mirroring and action understanding: an alternative account. In: P. Haggard, Y. Rosetti, M. Kawato, Sensorimotor Foundations of Higher Cognition. Attention and Performance XII, Oxford, Oxford University Press 2007 (pp. 453-459).
De Vega M. (2008). Levels of embodied meaning. From pointing to counterfactuals. In: De Vega M., Glenberg A.M., Graesser A.C., Symbols, Embodiment, and Meaning. Oxford, Oxford University Press 2008 (pp. 285-308).
https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199217274.003.0014
Fadiga L., Craighero L., Buccino G., Rizzolatti G. (2002). Speech listening specifically modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: A TMS study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 15(2), 399-402.
https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01874.x
Fargier, R., Paulignan, Y., Boulenger, V., Monaghan, P., Reboul, A., & Nazir, T. A. (2012). Learning to associate novel words with motor actions: Language-induced motor activity following short training. Cortex, 48(7), 888-899.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.07.003
Fischer M.H., Zwaan R.A. (2008). Embodied language: a review of the role of the motor system in language comprehension. Quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 61(6), 825-850.
https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210701623605
Fogassi L., Ferrari P. F., Gesierich B., Rozzi S., Chersi F., Rizzolatti G. (2005). Parietal lobe: from action organization to intention understanding. Science, 302, 662-667.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1106138
Gallese V. (2003). A neuroscientific grasp of concepts: from control to representation. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B, 358, 1231-1240.
https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2003.1315
Gallese V. (2007). Before and below ‘theory of mind’: embodied simulation and the neural correlates of social cognition. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B., 362(1480), 659-669.
https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2006.2002
Gallese V., Glenberg A.M. (2012). Action-based language: a theory of language acquisition, comprehension, and production. Cortex, 48(7), 905-922.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.010
Gallese V., Lakoff G. (2005). The brain’s concepts: the role of the sensory-motor system in reason and language. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 22, 455-479.
https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290442000310
Gentilucci M., Bernardis P., Crisi G., Volta R. D. (2006). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of Broca’s area affects verbal responses to gesture observation. J. Cogn. Neurosci., 18, 1059-1074.
https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.7.1059
Gentilucci M., Santunione P., Roy A. C., Stefanini S. (2004a). Execution and observation of bringing a fruit to the mouth affect syllable pronunciation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, 190-202.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03104.x
Gerson, S. A., Bekkering, H., & Hunnius, S. (2015). Short-term motor training, but not observational training, alters neurocognitive mechanisms of action processing in infancy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(6), 1207-1214.
https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00774
Glenberg A.M., Kaschak M.P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9(3), 558-565.
https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196313
Glenberg A.M., Robertson D.A. (1999) Indexical understanding of instructions. Discourse Processes, 28, 1-26.
https://doi.org/10.1080/01638539909545067
Glenberg A.M., Sato M., Cattaneo L. (2008). Use-induced motor plasticity affects the processing of abstract and concrete language. Current Biology, 18(7), R290-R291.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.036
Goldin-Meadow S. (1999). The role of gesture in communication and thinking. Trends Cogn. Sci., 3, 419-429.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01397-2
Hauk O., Johnsrude I., Pulvermueller F. (2004). Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron 41, 301-307.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00838-9
Hafri A., Trueswell J.C., Epstein R.A. (2017). Neural representations of observed actions generalize across static and dynamic visual input. J. Neurosci. 37, 3056-3071.
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2496-16.2017
Hewes, G.W. (1973). Primate communication and the gestural origins of language. Current Anthropology 14, 5-24.
https://doi.org/10.1086/201401
Heyes, C. (2010). Where do mirror neurons come from?. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(4), 575-583.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.007
Hickok G. (2009). Eight problems for the mirror neuron theory of action understanding in monkeys and humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 1229-1243.
https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21189
Huttenlocher J., Smiley P., Charney R. (1983). Emergence of action categories in the child: Evidence from verb meanings. Psychological Review, 90(1), 72-93.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.90.1.72
Iacoboni M., Molnar-Szakacs I., Gallese V., Buccino G., Mazziotta J., Rizzolatti G. (2005). Grasping the intentions of others with one’s owns mirror neuron system. PloS Biol. 3, 529-535.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030079
Jacob P., Jeannerod M. (2005). The motor theory of social cognition: A critique. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 21-25.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.11.003
Kendon, A. (2011). Some modern considerations for thinking about language evolution: a discussion of the evolution of language by Tecumseh Fitch. Public J. Semiotics 3(1), 79-108.
https://doi.org/10.37693/pjos.2011.3.8832
Libero, L.E., Maximo, J.O., Deshpande, H.D., Klinger, L.G., Klinger, M.R., Kana, R.K. (2014). The role of mirroring and mentalizing networks in mediating action intentions in autism. Mol. Autism 5 (1), 50.
https://doi.org/10.1186/2040-2392-5-50
Martin A., Wiggs C.L., Ungerleider L.G., Haxby J.V. (1996). Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge. Nature, 379, 649-652.
https://doi.org/10.1038/379649a0
Masataka N. (2001). Why early linguistic milestones are delayed in children with Williams syndrome: late onset of hand banging as a possible rate-limiting constraint on the emergence of canonical babbling. Devel. Sci. 4, 158-164.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00161
McGuigan F. J., Dollins A. B. (1989). Patterns of covert speech behavior and phonetic coding. Pavlovian J. Biol. Sci. 24, 19-26.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02964527
McNeill, D. (2012). How Language Began: Gesture and Speech in Human Evolution. Cambridge University. Press, Cambridge.
https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139108669
Mikulan E.P., Reynaldo L., Ibanez A. (2014). Homuncular mirrors: misunderstanding causality in embodied cognition. Front. Hum. Neurosci., 8, 1-4.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00299
Morey, R. D., Kaschak, M. P., Díez-Álamo, A. M., Glenberg, A. M., Zwaan, R. A., Lakens, D., … & Ziv-Crispel, N. (2021). A pre-registered, multi-lab non-replication of the action-sentence compatibility effect (ACE). Psychonomic bulletin & review, 1-14.
Nicholson T., Roser M., Bach P. (2017). Understanding the goals of everyday instrumental actions is primarily linked to object, not motor-kinematic, information: Evidence from fMRI. PLoS One, 12, 1-21.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169700
Paternoster A., Calzavarini F. (2020). Comprendere il linguaggio. Il mulino.
Paulus, M., Hunnius, S., Van Elk, M., & Bekkering, H. (2012). How learning to shake a rattle affects 8-month-old infants’ perception of the rattle’s sound: electrophysiological evidence for action-effect binding in infancy. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2(1), 90-96.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2011.05.006
Paulus, M., Hunnius, S., & Bekkering, H. (2013). Neurocognitive mechanisms underlying social learning in infancy: infants’ neural processing of the effects of others’ actions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(7), 774-779.
https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss065
Poizner H., Klima E.S., Bellugi, U. (1987). What the hands reveal about the brain, Cambridge MA, MIT Press.
Preissl H., Pulvermuller F., Lutzenberger W., Birbaumer N. (1995). Evoked potentials distinguish between nouns and verbs. Neuroscience Letters, 197, 81-83.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3940(95)11892-Z
Premack D., Woodruff G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515-526.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076512
Pulverman, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., Pruden, S., & Salkind, S. (2006). Conceptual foundations for verb learning: Celebrating the event. Action meets word: How children learn verbs, 2010, 134-159.
https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195170009.003.0006
Pulvermuller F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 576-582.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1706
Pulvermuller F. (2008). Grounding language in the brain. In: De Vega M., Glenberg A.M., Graesser A.C., Symbols, Embodiment, and Meaning. Oxford, Oxford University Press 2008 (pp. 85-116).
Pulvermuller F., Lutzenberger W., Preissl H. (1999). Nouns and verbs in the intact brain:
Evidence from event-related potentials and high-frequency cortical responses. Cerebral Cortex, 9, 498-508.
Pulvermuller F., Shtyrov Y., Ilmoniemi R. (2005). Brain signatures of meaning access in action word recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 884-892.
https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929054021111
Rizzolatti G., Arbib M.A. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences, 21(5), 188-194.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(98)01260-0
Rizzolatti G., Craighero L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169-192.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230
Rizzolatti G., Fogassi L., Gallese V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nat.Rev. Neurosci., 2, 661-670.
https://doi.org/10.1038/35090060
Salo, V. C., Ferrari, P. F., & Fox, N. A. (2019). The role of the motor system in action understanding and communication: Evidence from human infants and non‐human primates. Developmental psychobiology, 61(3), 390-401.
https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21779
Sandler, W. (2013). Vive la différence: sign language and spoken language in language evolution. Lang. Cogn. 5(2-3), 189-203.
https://doi.org/10.1515/langcog-2013-0013
Scott-Phillips T. (2015). Speaking our minds, Palgrave Macmillan (trad. it. Di’ quello che hai in mente, Roma, Carocci 2017).
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-137-31273-0
Shannon C.E., Weaver W. (1949). The mathematical theory of communication, Illinois, The University of Illinois Press.
Sperber D., Wilson D. (1986). Relevance: Communication and Cognition, Oxford, Blackwell.
Spunt R.P., Adolphs R. (2014). Validating the why/how contrast for functional MRI studies of theory of mind. Neuroimage, 99, 301-311.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.023
Spunt R.P., Lieberman M.D. (2013). The busy social brain: evidence for automaticity and control in the neural systems supporting social cognition and action understanding. Psychol. Sci., 24, 80-86.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612450884
Stokoe, W. C. (2002). Language in hand: Why sign came before speech. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.
Tettamanti M. et al. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 273-281.
https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929053124965
Thompson E.L., Bird G., Catmur C. (2019a). Mirror neurons, action understanding and social interaction: implications for educational neuroscience. Conference Abstract: 4th International Conference on Educational Neuroscience.
https://doi.org/10.3389/conf.fnhum.2019.229.00012
Thompson, E. L., Bird, G., & Catmur, C. (2019b). Conceptualizing and testing action understanding. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 105, 106-114.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.08.002
Tomasello, M. (2008). The origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/7551.001.0001
Wilson D., Sperber D. (2002). Relevance theory. G. Ward, L. Horn. Handbook of Pragmatics, Blackwell.
Wolpert D.M., Kawato M. (1998). Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control. Neural Networks, 11(7), 1317-1329.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0893-6080(98)00066-5
Wurm M.F., Ariani G., Greenlee M.W., Lingnau A. (2016). Decoding concrete and abstract action representations during explicit and implicit conceptual processing. Cereb. Cortex, 26, 3390-3401.
https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv169
Wurm M.F., Lingnau A., (2015). Decoding actions at different levels of abstraction. J. Neurosci., 35, 7727-7735.
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0188-15.2015
Zywiczynski, P., Gontier, N., & Wacewicz, S. (2017). The evolution of (proto-) language: Focus on mechanisms.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2017.06.004

Comments are closed.