Avant, Vol. XIII, No. 1, https://doi.org/10.26913/ava202204
published under license CC BY-NC-ND 3.0
Angelo D. Delliponti
Academia Copernicana,
Nicolaus Copernicus University
angelo.d.delliponti@gmail.com
Received 9 September 2021; accepted 19 April 2022; published 17 June 2022.
Download full text
Abstract: The ostensive-inferential model is a model of communication, an alternative to the code model of communication, based on pragmatic competence: it explains human communication in terms of expression and recognition of informative and communicative intentions, founding comprehension on the distinction between literal meaning and the speaker’s meaning. Through informative intentions we try to make evident the content of a message to a receiver, or to make evident what we want to communicate to him/her: communicative intentions are used to make evident the very fact that we intend to communicate. One hypothesis is that ostensive-inferential communication is what makes human language possible. Since an extensive literature has highlighted the role of the Theory of Mind in ostensive-inferential communication, this hypothesis fits with the idea that a mechanism for mentalizing underlies human communication. The aim of the present paper is to stress the role of lower-level mechanisms, specifically of motor simulation, in the recognition of informative and communicative intentions, in order to outline an embodied account of ostensive communication. Specifically, the hypothesis is that this process is involved in language acquisition during development, and that it plays a role in the associative learning process involved in language acquisition during childhood. To this aim, in future research it may be useful to test the involvement of motor simulation (specifically, phono-articulatory and semantic) in the recognition of informative and communicative intentions in toddlers. Since some models of language evolution focus on the role of motor simulation, a supplementary goal is to deepen its role in the biological evolution of language, focusing on the specific link between motor simulation and intentions in the framework of ostensive-inferential model.
Keywords: ostensive communication; motor simulation; communicative intention; informative intention; embodiment; language acquisition
Bibliography
Angrave L.C., Glenberg A.M. (2007). Infant Gestures Predict Verb Production One Year Later. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/e705442007-001 |
||||
Antognini K., Daum M. M. (2019) . Toddlers show sensorimotor activity during auditory verb processing. Neuropsychologia, 126, 82-91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.07.022 |
||||
Apperly I.A. (2011). Mindreaders: the cognitive basis of “theory of mind”. Psychology Press, New York. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203833926 |
||||
Arbib M.A. (2012). How the brain got language: The mirror system hypothesis, New York (NY), Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:osobl/9780199896684.001.0001 |
||||
Arbib M.A. (2013). Complex imitation and the language-ready brain. Language and Cognition, 5, 273-312. https://doi.org/10.1515/langcog-2013-0020 |
||||
Arbib M.A., Rizzolatti G. (1997). Neural expectations: A possible evolutionary path from manual skills to language. Communication and Cognition, 29, 393-424. | ||||
Armstrong, D. F., Stokoe,W. C., & Wilcox, S. E. (1995). Gesture and the nature of language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511620911 |
||||
Aziz-Zadeh L., Wilson S.M., Rizzolatti G., Iacoboni M. (2006). Embodied semantics and the premotor cortex: Congruent representations for visually presented actions and linguistic phrases describing actions. Current Biology, 16, 1818-1823. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.060 |
||||
Bak T.H., Hodges J.R. (2003). The effects of motor neurone disease on language: Further evidence. Brain and Language, 89, 354-361. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00357-2 |
||||
Bambini V. (2017). Il cervello pragmatico, Roma, Carocci. | ||||
Bernardis P., Gentilucci M. (2006). Speech and gesture share the same communication system. Neuropsychologia, 44, 178-190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.05.007 |
||||
Binkofski F., Buccino G. (2006). The role of ventral premotor cortex in action execution and action understanding. J. Physiol. Paris 99, 396-405. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2006.03.005 |
||||
Borghi A., Caruana F. (2016). Il cervello in azione, Bologna, Il Mulino. | ||||
Buccino G., Binkofski F., Fink G.R., Fadiga L., Fogassi L., Freund H.J., et al. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: An fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13(2), 400-404. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.2001.01385.x |
||||
Buccino G. Binkofski F., Riggio L. (2004b). The mirror neuron system and action recognition. Brain and Language, 89(2), 370-376. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00356-0 |
||||
Buccino G., Riggio L., Melli G., Binkofski F., Gallese V., Rizzolatti G. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: a combined TMS and behavioral study. Cogn. Brain Res. 24, 355-363. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.02.020 |
||||
Buccino G., Solodkin A., Small S. (2006). Functions of the mirror neuron system: Implications for neurorehabilitation. Cognitive Behavioral Neurology, 19, 55-63. https://doi.org/10.1097/00146965-200603000-00007 |
||||
Byom L. J. & Mutlu B. (2013). Theory of mind: Mechanisms, methods, and new directions. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 7, 413. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00413 |
||||
Cavallo, A., Lungu, O., Becchio, C., Ansuini, C., Rustichini, A., & Fadiga, L. (2015). When gaze opens the channel for communication: Integrative role of IFG and MPFC. NeuroImage, 119, 63-69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.025 |
||||
Chomsky N. (1957). Syntactic structures, Princeton, Mouton and Co. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783112316009 |
||||
Cole, E.J., Barraclough, N.E., Andrews, T.J. (2019). Reduced connectivity between mentalizing and mirror systems in autism spectrum condition. Neuropsychologia 122, 88-97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.11.008 |
||||
Cook R., Bird G., Catmur C., Press C., Heyes C. (2014). Mirror neurons: from origin to function. Behav. Brain Sci. 37, 177-192. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X13000903 |
||||
Cooper, R. P., Cook, R., Dickinson, A., & Heyes, C. M. (2013). Associative (not Hebbian) learning and the mirror neuron system. Neuroscience Letters, 540, 28-36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2012.10.002 |
||||
Corballis, M. C. (2002). From hand to mouth: The origins of language. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. https://doi.org/10.1515/9780691221731 |
||||
Csibra G. (2007). Action mirroring and action understanding: an alternative account. In: P. Haggard, Y. Rosetti, M. Kawato, Sensorimotor Foundations of Higher Cognition. Attention and Performance XII, Oxford, Oxford University Press 2007 (pp. 453-459). | ||||
De Vega M. (2008). Levels of embodied meaning. From pointing to counterfactuals. In: De Vega M., Glenberg A.M., Graesser A.C., Symbols, Embodiment, and Meaning. Oxford, Oxford University Press 2008 (pp. 285-308). https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199217274.003.0014 |
||||
Fadiga L., Craighero L., Buccino G., Rizzolatti G. (2002). Speech listening specifically modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: A TMS study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 15(2), 399-402. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01874.x |
||||
Fargier, R., Paulignan, Y., Boulenger, V., Monaghan, P., Reboul, A., & Nazir, T. A. (2012). Learning to associate novel words with motor actions: Language-induced motor activity following short training. Cortex, 48(7), 888-899. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.07.003 |
||||
Fischer M.H., Zwaan R.A. (2008). Embodied language: a review of the role of the motor system in language comprehension. Quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 61(6), 825-850. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210701623605 |
||||
Fogassi L., Ferrari P. F., Gesierich B., Rozzi S., Chersi F., Rizzolatti G. (2005). Parietal lobe: from action organization to intention understanding. Science, 302, 662-667. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1106138 |
||||
Gallese V. (2003). A neuroscientific grasp of concepts: from control to representation. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B, 358, 1231-1240. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2003.1315 |
||||
Gallese V. (2007). Before and below ‘theory of mind’: embodied simulation and the neural correlates of social cognition. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B., 362(1480), 659-669. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2006.2002 |
||||
Gallese V., Glenberg A.M. (2012). Action-based language: a theory of language acquisition, comprehension, and production. Cortex, 48(7), 905-922. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.010 |
||||
Gallese V., Lakoff G. (2005). The brain’s concepts: the role of the sensory-motor system in reason and language. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 22, 455-479. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290442000310 |
||||
Gentilucci M., Bernardis P., Crisi G., Volta R. D. (2006). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of Broca’s area affects verbal responses to gesture observation. J. Cogn. Neurosci., 18, 1059-1074. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.7.1059 |
||||
Gentilucci M., Santunione P., Roy A. C., Stefanini S. (2004a). Execution and observation of bringing a fruit to the mouth affect syllable pronunciation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, 190-202. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03104.x |
||||
Gerson, S. A., Bekkering, H., & Hunnius, S. (2015). Short-term motor training, but not observational training, alters neurocognitive mechanisms of action processing in infancy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(6), 1207-1214. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00774 |
||||
Glenberg A.M., Kaschak M.P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9(3), 558-565. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196313 |
||||
Glenberg A.M., Robertson D.A. (1999) Indexical understanding of instructions. Discourse Processes, 28, 1-26. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638539909545067 |
||||
Glenberg A.M., Sato M., Cattaneo L. (2008). Use-induced motor plasticity affects the processing of abstract and concrete language. Current Biology, 18(7), R290-R291. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.036 |
||||
Goldin-Meadow S. (1999). The role of gesture in communication and thinking. Trends Cogn. Sci., 3, 419-429. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01397-2 |
||||
Hauk O., Johnsrude I., Pulvermueller F. (2004). Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron 41, 301-307. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00838-9 |
||||
Hafri A., Trueswell J.C., Epstein R.A. (2017). Neural representations of observed actions generalize across static and dynamic visual input. J. Neurosci. 37, 3056-3071. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2496-16.2017 |
||||
Hewes, G.W. (1973). Primate communication and the gestural origins of language. Current Anthropology 14, 5-24. https://doi.org/10.1086/201401 |
||||
Heyes, C. (2010). Where do mirror neurons come from?. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(4), 575-583. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.007 |
||||
Hickok G. (2009). Eight problems for the mirror neuron theory of action understanding in monkeys and humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 1229-1243. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21189 |
||||
Huttenlocher J., Smiley P., Charney R. (1983). Emergence of action categories in the child: Evidence from verb meanings. Psychological Review, 90(1), 72-93. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.90.1.72 |
||||
Iacoboni M., Molnar-Szakacs I., Gallese V., Buccino G., Mazziotta J., Rizzolatti G. (2005). Grasping the intentions of others with one’s owns mirror neuron system. PloS Biol. 3, 529-535. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030079 |
||||
Jacob P., Jeannerod M. (2005). The motor theory of social cognition: A critique. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 21-25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.11.003 |
||||
Kendon, A. (2011). Some modern considerations for thinking about language evolution: a discussion of the evolution of language by Tecumseh Fitch. Public J. Semiotics 3(1), 79-108. https://doi.org/10.37693/pjos.2011.3.8832 |
||||
Libero, L.E., Maximo, J.O., Deshpande, H.D., Klinger, L.G., Klinger, M.R., Kana, R.K. (2014). The role of mirroring and mentalizing networks in mediating action intentions in autism. Mol. Autism 5 (1), 50. https://doi.org/10.1186/2040-2392-5-50 |
||||
Martin A., Wiggs C.L., Ungerleider L.G., Haxby J.V. (1996). Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge. Nature, 379, 649-652. https://doi.org/10.1038/379649a0 |
||||
Masataka N. (2001). Why early linguistic milestones are delayed in children with Williams syndrome: late onset of hand banging as a possible rate-limiting constraint on the emergence of canonical babbling. Devel. Sci. 4, 158-164. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00161 |
||||
McGuigan F. J., Dollins A. B. (1989). Patterns of covert speech behavior and phonetic coding. Pavlovian J. Biol. Sci. 24, 19-26. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02964527 |
||||
McNeill, D. (2012). How Language Began: Gesture and Speech in Human Evolution. Cambridge University. Press, Cambridge. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139108669 |
||||
Mikulan E.P., Reynaldo L., Ibanez A. (2014). Homuncular mirrors: misunderstanding causality in embodied cognition. Front. Hum. Neurosci., 8, 1-4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00299 |
||||
Morey, R. D., Kaschak, M. P., Díez-Álamo, A. M., Glenberg, A. M., Zwaan, R. A., Lakens, D., … & Ziv-Crispel, N. (2021). A pre-registered, multi-lab non-replication of the action-sentence compatibility effect (ACE). Psychonomic bulletin & review, 1-14. | ||||
Nicholson T., Roser M., Bach P. (2017). Understanding the goals of everyday instrumental actions is primarily linked to object, not motor-kinematic, information: Evidence from fMRI. PLoS One, 12, 1-21. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169700 |
||||
Paternoster A., Calzavarini F. (2020). Comprendere il linguaggio. Il mulino. | ||||
Paulus, M., Hunnius, S., Van Elk, M., & Bekkering, H. (2012). How learning to shake a rattle affects 8-month-old infants’ perception of the rattle’s sound: electrophysiological evidence for action-effect binding in infancy. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2(1), 90-96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2011.05.006 |
||||
Paulus, M., Hunnius, S., & Bekkering, H. (2013). Neurocognitive mechanisms underlying social learning in infancy: infants’ neural processing of the effects of others’ actions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(7), 774-779. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss065 |
||||
Poizner H., Klima E.S., Bellugi, U. (1987). What the hands reveal about the brain, Cambridge MA, MIT Press. | ||||
Preissl H., Pulvermuller F., Lutzenberger W., Birbaumer N. (1995). Evoked potentials distinguish between nouns and verbs. Neuroscience Letters, 197, 81-83. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3940(95)11892-Z |
||||
Premack D., Woodruff G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515-526. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076512 |
||||
Pulverman, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., Pruden, S., & Salkind, S. (2006). Conceptual foundations for verb learning: Celebrating the event. Action meets word: How children learn verbs, 2010, 134-159. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195170009.003.0006 |
||||
Pulvermuller F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 576-582. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1706 |
||||
Pulvermuller F. (2008). Grounding language in the brain. In: De Vega M., Glenberg A.M., Graesser A.C., Symbols, Embodiment, and Meaning. Oxford, Oxford University Press 2008 (pp. 85-116). | ||||
Pulvermuller F., Lutzenberger W., Preissl H. (1999). Nouns and verbs in the intact brain: | ||||
Evidence from event-related potentials and high-frequency cortical responses. Cerebral Cortex, 9, 498-508. | ||||
Pulvermuller F., Shtyrov Y., Ilmoniemi R. (2005). Brain signatures of meaning access in action word recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 884-892. https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929054021111 |
||||
Rizzolatti G., Arbib M.A. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences, 21(5), 188-194. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(98)01260-0 |
||||
Rizzolatti G., Craighero L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169-192. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230 |
||||
Rizzolatti G., Fogassi L., Gallese V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nat.Rev. Neurosci., 2, 661-670. https://doi.org/10.1038/35090060 |
||||
Salo, V. C., Ferrari, P. F., & Fox, N. A. (2019). The role of the motor system in action understanding and communication: Evidence from human infants and non‐human primates. Developmental psychobiology, 61(3), 390-401. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21779 |
||||
Sandler, W. (2013). Vive la différence: sign language and spoken language in language evolution. Lang. Cogn. 5(2-3), 189-203. https://doi.org/10.1515/langcog-2013-0013 |
||||
Scott-Phillips T. (2015). Speaking our minds, Palgrave Macmillan (trad. it. Di’ quello che hai in mente, Roma, Carocci 2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-137-31273-0 |
||||
Shannon C.E., Weaver W. (1949). The mathematical theory of communication, Illinois, The University of Illinois Press. | ||||
Sperber D., Wilson D. (1986). Relevance: Communication and Cognition, Oxford, Blackwell. | ||||
Spunt R.P., Adolphs R. (2014). Validating the why/how contrast for functional MRI studies of theory of mind. Neuroimage, 99, 301-311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.023 |
||||
Spunt R.P., Lieberman M.D. (2013). The busy social brain: evidence for automaticity and control in the neural systems supporting social cognition and action understanding. Psychol. Sci., 24, 80-86. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612450884 |
||||
Stokoe, W. C. (2002). Language in hand: Why sign came before speech. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press. | ||||
Tettamanti M. et al. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 273-281. https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929053124965 |
||||
Thompson E.L., Bird G., Catmur C. (2019a). Mirror neurons, action understanding and social interaction: implications for educational neuroscience. Conference Abstract: 4th International Conference on Educational Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.3389/conf.fnhum.2019.229.00012 |
||||
Thompson, E. L., Bird, G., & Catmur, C. (2019b). Conceptualizing and testing action understanding. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 105, 106-114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.08.002 |
||||
Tomasello, M. (2008). The origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/7551.001.0001 |
||||
Wilson D., Sperber D. (2002). Relevance theory. G. Ward, L. Horn. Handbook of Pragmatics, Blackwell. | ||||
Wolpert D.M., Kawato M. (1998). Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control. Neural Networks, 11(7), 1317-1329. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0893-6080(98)00066-5 |
||||
Wurm M.F., Ariani G., Greenlee M.W., Lingnau A. (2016). Decoding concrete and abstract action representations during explicit and implicit conceptual processing. Cereb. Cortex, 26, 3390-3401. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv169 |
||||
Wurm M.F., Lingnau A., (2015). Decoding actions at different levels of abstraction. J. Neurosci., 35, 7727-7735. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0188-15.2015 |
||||
Zywiczynski, P., Gontier, N., & Wacewicz, S. (2017). The evolution of (proto-) language: Focus on mechanisms. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2017.06.004 |