Integration of Processes in the Study of Insight and Innovation

Thea Ionescu, Alexandra Marian, Paula Moldovan, Beatrix Perde, Roxana Vescan, Calin Hopsitar, Doris Rogobete, Ligia Suciu

Abstract


Humans are known to be able to solve problems creatively, but how exactly this occurs is still a matter of debate. Insight problem solving is one important way to study creativity in adults, but studies that use this method with preschool children are rare. In this paper, we present two studies: one in which the problem is solved with known objects (participants were preschool children aged 4 to 7 years) and one in which new objects were created (participants were children aged 5 to 6 years and adults). For both studies, results are discussed in relation to the various processes that can explain insight problem solving. We argue that the integration of these processes under the overarching variability–stability–flexibility pattern might be a better way to investigate insight and ultimately to understand innovation.

Keywords


innovation; insight; novelty

Full Text:

PDF

References


Adamson, R. E., & Taylor, D. W. (1954). Functional fixedness as related to elapsed time and to set. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47(2), 122–126. doi:10.1037/h0057297

Ayman-Nolley, S. (1992). Vygotsky’s perspective on the development of imagination and creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 5(1), 77–85, doi:10.1080/10400419209534424

Beck, S. R., Williams, C., Cutting, N., Apperly, I. A., & Chappell, J. (2016). Individual differences in children’s innovative problem-solving are not predicted by divergent thinking or executive functions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1690), 1–11. doi:10.1098/rstb.2015.0190

Bergen, D. (2002). The role of pretend play in children’s cognitive development. Early Childhood Research & Practice, 4(1). Retrieved from http://ecrp.uiuc.edu/v4n1/bergen.html

Cain, K. (2010). Reading development and difficulties: An introduction, Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.

Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72(4), 1032–1053. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00333

Carr, K., Kendal, R. L., & Flynn, E. G. (2016). Eureka! What is innovation, how does it develop, and who does it? Child Development, 87(5), 1505–1519. doi:10.1111/cdev.12549

Clark, P. M., Griffing, P. S., & Johnson, L. G. (1989). Symbolic play and ideational fluency as aspects of the evolving divergent cognitive style in young children. Early Child Development and Care, 51(1), 77–88, doi:10.1080/0300443890510107

Dansky, J. L. (1980). Make-believe: A mediator of the relationship between play and associative fluency. Child Development, 51(2), 576–579. doi:10.2307/1129296

Defeyter, M. A., & German, T. P. (2003). Acquiring an understanding of design: Evidence from children’s insight problem solving. Cognition, 89(2), 133–155. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00098-2

German, T. P., & Defeyter, M. A. (2000). Immunity to functional fixedness in young children. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7(4), 707–712.

German, T. P., & Johnson, S. C. (2002). Function and the origins of the design stance. Journal of Cognition and Development, 3(3), 279–300. doi:10.1207/S15327647JCD0303_2

Glăveanu, V. P. (2010). Paradigms in the study of creativity: Introducing the perspective of cultural psychology. New Ideas in Psychology, 28(1), 79–93. doi:10.1016/j.newideapsych.2009.07.007

Glăveanu, V. P. (2011). Children and creativity: A most (un)likely pair? Thinking Skills and Creativity, 6(2), 122–131. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2011.03.002

Gummerum, M., Denham, S. L. (2014). Cognitive innovation: From cell to society. Europe's Journal of Psychology, 10(4), 586–588. doi:10.5964/ejop.v10i4.879

Hélie, S., & Sun, R. (2010). Incubation, insight, and creative problem solving: A unified theory and a connectionist model. Psychological Review, 117(3), 994–1024. doi:10.1037/a0019532

Ionescu, T. (2012). Exploring the nature of cognitive flexibility. New Ideas in Psychology, 30(2), 190–200. doi:10.1016/j.newideapsych.2011.11.001

Ionescu, T. (2017). The variability–stability–flexibility pattern: A possible key to understanding the flexibility of the human mind. Review of General Psychology, 21(2), 123–131. doi:10.1037/gpr0000110

Kemple, K. M., & Nissenberg, S. A. (2000). Nurturing creativity in early childhood education: Families are part of it. Early Childhood Education Journal, 28(1), 67–71.

Klein, G., & Jarosz, A. (2011). A naturalistic study of insight. Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making, 5(4), 335–351. doi:10.1177/1555343411427013

Lane, J. D., Ronfard, S., Francioli, S. P., & Harris, P. L. (2016). Children’s imagination and belief: Prone to flights of fancy or grounded in reality? Cognition, 152, 127–140. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2016.03.022

Lillard, A. S., Lerner, M. D., Hopkins, E. J., Dore, R. A., Smith, E. D., & Palmquist, C. M. (2012). The impact of pretend play on children’s development: A review of the evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 139(1), 1–34. doi:10.1037/a0029321

Magid, R. W., Sheskin, M., & Schulz, L. E. (2015). Imagination and the generation of new ideas. Cognitive Development, 34, 99–110. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.12.008

Mok, L. W. (2014). The interplay between spontaneous and controlled processing in creative cognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00663

Morasch, K. C., & Bell, M. A. (2011). The role of inhibitory control in behavioral and physiological expressions of toddler executive function. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 593–606, doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.07.003

Müller, U., & Kerns, K. (2015). The development of executive function. In L. S. Liben & U. Müller (Vol. Eds.), R. M. Lerner (Series Ed.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Cognitive processes (7th ed., pp. 571–623). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley. doi:10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy214

Nowak-Fabrykowski, K. (1994). Can symbolic play prepare children for their future? Early Child Development and Care, 102(1), 63–69, doi:10.1080/0300443941020104

Ormerod, T. C., MacGregor, J. N., & Chronicle, E. P. (2002). Dynamics and constraints in insight problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(4), 791–799. doi:10.1037//0278-7393.28.4.791

Pellegrini, A. D. (1984). The effects of exploration and play on young children’s associative fluency. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 4(1), 29–40. doi:10.2190/0W5N-X8E8-WKWF-9A1R

Reuland, E. (2016). Language and imagination: Evolutionary explorations. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 1–24, Advance online publication. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.12.017

Russ, S. W., & Wallace, C. E. (2013). Pretend play and creative processes. American Journal of Play, 6(1), 136–148.

Schachar, R., & Logan, G. D. (1990). Impulsivity and inhibitory control in normal development and childhood psychopathology. Developmental Psychology, 26(5), 710–720.

Siegler, R. S. (1999). Strategic development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(11), 430–435. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01372-8

Simonton, D. K. (2016). Creativity, automaticity, irrationality, fortuity, fantasy, and other contingencies: An eightfold response typology. Review of General Psychology, 20(2), 194–204. doi:10.1037/gpr0000075

Williams, B. R., Ponesse, J. S., Schachar, R. J., Logan, G. D., & Tannock, R. (1999). Development of inhibitory control across the life span. Developmental Psychology, 35(1), 205–213.

Yonge, G. D. (1966). Structure of experience and functional fixedness. Journal of Educational Psychology, 57(2), 115–120. doi:10.1037/h0022967


Refbacks

  • There are currently no refbacks.


Copyright (c) 2018 Thea Ionescu, Alexandra Marian, Paula Moldovan, Beatrix Perde, Roxana Vescan, Calin Hopsitar, Doris Rogobete, Ligia Suciu